Subscribe to Annie's Substack

“Hazardous Waste: We Bury Our Mistakes” — Annie’s Newsletter, April 5, 2019

HAZARDOUS WASTE: WE BURY OUR MISTAKES
The dangers of selective memory

THE RACE FOR HAPPINESS IS FREQUENTLY RUN ON A TREADMILL
The perils of benchmarking

WHAT “GAMING THE SYSTEM” TELLS US ABOUT THE MERIT OF THE SYSTEM
The college-admissions scandal and the future of standardized testing

KEITH STANOVICH’S LATEST CONTRIBUTION TO OPEN-MINDED THINKING:
A brave admission of lack of viewpoint diversity in his own research

HAZARDOUS WASTE: WE BURY OUR MISTAKES

The dangers of selective memory

Our ability to learn from experience is only as good as our ability to accurately remember that experience.

It turns out our memory is selective. We remember good outcomes and forget bad ones.

Thanks to a tweet from Carnegie Mellon behavioral economist Alex Imas, I learned of a new study by Katrin Gödker, Peiran Jiao, and Paul Smeets about how selective memory affects investing decisions

“Subjects who previously invested in a risky stock are more likely to remember positive investment outcomes and less likely to remember negative outcomes…. After investing, they form overly optimistic beliefs and re-invest in the stock even when doing so reduces their expected return.”

According to the authors, this is the first empirical evidence for a systematic memory bias in financial decisions.

It certainly makes sense, and it’s not just confined to financial decisions.

We are all motivated to create a positive self-narrative. And that creates a tension.

In the long run, if we remember our wins and losses accurately, our decisions will improve, leading to better outcomes and a better overall self-narrative.

In the long run.

But the catch is that we have to take multiple hits to our self-narrative to get there.

Losing feels like making a mistake. Winning feels like, well, winning. So we remember the wins to get that short-term narrative boost and forget the losses to avoid the narrative ding.

That’s the tension. That’s why we bury our mistakes.

THE RACE FOR HAPPINESS IS FREQUENTLY RUN ON A TREADMILL

The perils of benchmarking

Morgan Housel has a spot on my must-read list and one of his latest posts, “Death, Taxes, and a Few Other Things,” doesn’t disappoint, especially because it is about a topic I have been obsessed with lately: Benchmarks.

(In February, I wrote about another excellent article on benchmarking, Dan Egan’s “How to lose by beating the (wrong) benchmark.”)

Housel points out that, adjusting for inflation, American families are doing much better than in the 1950s or 1960s.

Yet, there is a widespread belief that the fifties and sixties were the peak era for the middle class. Nostalgia for how good everyone had it back then is a prominent part of our political discourse.

This despite data demonstrating otherwise.

“Median family income adjusted for inflation was $29,000 in 1955. In 1965 it was $42,000. Today it’s just over $62,000.”

So what gives?

As Housel says, “A perpetually moving goalpost” is what gives.

“Part of the disconnect between feelings and reality can be explained by the shift in expectations over the last 60 years. To generalize only a little: In the 1950s camping was an acceptable vacation. Hand-me-downs were acceptable clothes. A 983 square foot house was an acceptable size. Kids sharing a room was an acceptable arrangement. A tire swing was acceptable entertainment. Few of those things would be acceptable baselines by most households today.”

That sounds like a benchmarking problem to me.

If we benchmarked against what was acceptable in the fifties and sixties, we would all be ecstatic. We’d be like, “Holy cow! I have a computer in my hand and a polio vaccine!”

But we don’t. We are relative pricers. Relative to our reference group. Relative to what our neighbors have. Relative to what we see on Instagram.

“Relative” means that by the time we arrive at our initial goal, the goalposts have moved.

This is the problem with thinking that offering someone the data showing that they are better off will solve it.

Data doesn’t care about your feelings. But the obverse of that is that your feelings don’t care about the data.

We can track all we want along the axis of time. But people judge how they are doing not in comparison to the past but in comparison to the now.

The irony is that as we make progress, even if people’s lives get objectively better, their happiness can’t catch up because that progress changes the benchmark.

(I actually have another piece from Housel I’d like to share and will try to get to it in the next newsletter. This is assuming he doesn’t write something in the immediate future I’ll want to make an even higher priority to share. The takeaway: check out his blog regularly.)

WHAT “GAMING THE SYSTEM” TELLS US ABOUT THE MERIT OF THE SYSTEM

The college-admissions scandal and the future of standardized testing

On March 12, federal prosecutors in Boston charged fifty people in a conspiracy to influence college-admissions decisions at eight schools. Defendants include numerous wealthy and well-known people who allegedly paid a college-admissions consultant to get their children into college.

The highest-profile defendants in the case so far are television actresses Felicity Huffman and Lori Laughlin. If we’re going to prejudge the case, I can see throwing Felicity Huffman under the bus – she was one of the Desperate Housewives, so connect the dots, right? – but Lori Laughlin from Full/Fuller House?

Maybe if it was Uncle Jesse, with his devil-may-care attitude, preoccupation with his hair, and insistence on living in his widowed brother’s attic.

But et tu, Aunt Becky? Et tu?

Because the defendants’ alleged conduct involved cheating on standardized tests (like the SAT and ACT) by, among other things, paying proctors to change answers and hiring stand-ins to take the tests, this has also created a backlash against standardized tests.

The immediate reaction to the recent scandal has been to move the needle towards eliminating standardized testing altogether.

New York Times article by Eliza Shapiro and Dana Goldstein titled, “Is the College Cheating Scandal the ‘Final Straw’ for Standardized Tests?” summarized the sentiment:

“The degrees to which rich and famous families may have gone to cheat on them could become a watershed moment for the rejection of standardized tests at every level of the education system – but particularly college admissions.”

I think the conclusions that many are drawing, that the cheating shows we need to eliminate standardized testing, is exactly backwards.

What does it say that the advantages of wealth, access, and celebrity aren’t, alone, enough to “beat” grades and standardized tests as college gatekeepers? What does it say that wealthy, famous parents also have to commit crimes to get the outcome they want from the college admissions process?

To me, that says objective measures like grades and standardized tests must be working toward their intended goal, to make sure that more people are getting into college based on merit rather than access.

Jay Van Bavel started an interesting Twitter thread about standardized tests in college admissions that bolsters this conclusion. On March 22, he tweeted:

Regardless of whether you agree with Van Bavel, you should read his observations throughout the thread.

(In fact, if you disagree with him, it’s probably more important that you read it because it will help you calibrate your position – or strengthen it if you can address his defense of the system.)

His fundamental point is that the tests were designed to increase social mobility through education. Although they aren’t perfect at accomplishing it, they are more fair than other measures.

In fact, a system that focuses on subjective, individualized factors like extracurricular activities unquestionably gives higher-income families a massive advantage.

This table on CivicYouth.org illustrates how wealthier families outspend poorer families on extracurricular activities 8.5-to-1, and that disparity has been growing:

That’s an advantage purely based on financial circumstances. And there’s no intentional, fraudulent behavior necessary to get that advantage.

Of course, one could argue that money doesn’t just buy extracurriculars but also buys test prep. It is true that it does but, despite anecdotal claims by some tutors, “there is no peer-reviewed scientific evidence,” according to Van Bavel, “that coaching can reliably provide more than a modest boost.”

In addition, income inequality in test preparation is something that can be constructively addressed – and already is. Khan Academy, for example, provides free personalized practice recommendations for SAT skills, plus eight official practice exams from the College Board.

Shapiro and Goldstein noted in their Times article that free test prep is expanding:

“Kaplan now livestreams some prep sessions for free, and some school districts provide free prep courses. In 2017, the College Board, which administers the SAT, and Khan Academy, an education app, announced that students who used a free service saw gains on their scores.”

Until colleges start counting a job in a fast-food restaurant as an extracurricular, it seems to me that it’s easier to address income-based inequalities connected with standardized testing than in extracurriculars.

If we take away standardized tests, the advantage of wealth becomes greater and more difficult to address.

The system clearly isn’t perfect, but if it takes such extraordinary lengths to beat the system, it must have some merit.

The testing and admissions processes should improve at preventing fraud and also continue to address the potential for structural inequalities.

KEITH STANOVICH’S LATEST CONTRIBUTION TO OPEN-MINDED THINKING:

A brave admission of lack of viewpoint diversity in his own research

What do you do when you devote your career to measuring rationality and discover one of your measures is biased? AND that the bias was caused by a lack of viewpoint diversity in your research group?

You do what Keith Stanovich did: go public with the shortcoming in your measure and highlight your group’s lack of viewpoint diversity as a case study.

Stanovich is an emeritus professor of psychology at the University of Toronto and a proponent of active open-mindedness. He and colleagues developed questionnaires and scales for measuring “actively open-minded thinking” (AOT).

AOT is a gauge of willingness to consider alternative opinions, sensitivity to evidence conflicting with current beliefs, and reflective thought.

Obviously, such work is of great importance to decision science.

A controversial result of his AOT work was the finding of a high negative correlation between open-mindedness and religiosity.

Stanovich and Maggie Toplak, according to the abstract of their article for the June 2019 issue of Cognition, isolated the AOT item responsible for this conclusion.

“To our consternation, we realized that it was our research team that had introduced these items to skew correlations …. We believe this lesson in item construction resulted from the lack of intellectual diversity in our own laboratory (specifically, the overwhelmingly secular composition of our own lab personnel).”

The scientific method requires this kind of intellectual honesty, but it’s still rare in practice.

Phil Tetlock tweeted about the importance of Stanovich’s recognition of this bias:

For twenty years, this item has been part of his AOT questionnaire. It’s reasonable to say his AOT work is part of his identity as a scientist.

It’s a great example of truth-seeking that he was able to recognize this, publicly admit the error, and encourage others to learn from it.

This is what it means to embrace updating and calibrating beliefs and embracing being wrong in pursuit of the truth.

HOW I DECIDE’S POKER BALL

April 27, The Union League in Philadelphia, and you’re invited!

How I Decide, the nonprofit I cofounded to improve the lives of youth through Decision Education, is holding its Poker Ball on Saturday, April 27, 2019, at the Union League in Philadelphia.

It promises to be a fun evening, raising money for decision education: music, magic, entertainment, cocktails, dining, and a No-Limit Texas Hold ‘Em tournament. Raffle, auction, and tournament prizes include one-of-a-kind experiences, luxury watches, and six hours of private jet time (thanks to Apollo Jets).

The website has further information about the Poker Ballattending and contributing, and, of course, about How I Decide and how it’s working to achieve its goals.

THIS WEEK’S VISUAL ILLUSION
From Akiyoshi Kitaokaconcentric circles that appear to be a spiral